A Fala dirigida à criança em português brasileiro:

A prosódia de perguntas

Autores

  • Maria Cristina Lobo Name UFJF
  • Juan Manuel Sosa UFJF

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-460x202258879

Resumo

A fala dirigida à criança (FDC) é o registro comumente usado nas interações bebê-adulto em muitas comunidades linguísticas, e apresenta uma série de particularidades lexicais, sintáticas e prosódicas quando comparada à fala entre adultos, tais como palavras com reduplicação de sílabas, alta taxa de perguntas e prosódia exagerada. Neste estudo, elegemos como objeto de investigação as interrogativas e suas propriedades prosódicas. A análise de dados de interações de dez bebês de 4 a 12 meses adquirindo o português brasileiro e seus cuidadores revelou que as perguntas corresponderam a 33% do total de enunciados produzidos e são realçadas principalmente pelo pitch marcado (maior pitch range e pitch span), e maior duração. Ainda, os contornos de pitch das perguntas Qu e Sim/Não (perguntas totais) seguem seus respectivos padrões observados na fala adulta. Esses resultados são compatíveis com os encontrados na literatura e apontam que, de modo geral, A FDC usada no português brasileiro apresenta características semelhantes a esse registro em outras línguas.

Referências

Adriaans, F., & Swingley, D. (2017). Prosodic exaggeration within infant- directed speech: Consequences for vowel learnability. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141(5), 3070-3078. https://doi. org/10.1121/1.4982246.

Barbosa, P. A. (2000). “Syllable-timing in Brazilian Portuguese”: Uma crítica a Roy Major. DELTA, 16(2), 369-402. https://doi. org/10.1590/S0102-44502000000200006.

Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R. (2021). Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292-325. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698.

Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2019). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.1.08) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org.

Braga, G., Frota, S., & Fernandes-Svartman, F. (2022). Guinea-Bissau Portuguese: What the intonation of yes-no question shows about this variety. Special issue Prosody and interfaces (Ed. by C. Serra, F. Fernandes-Svartman, and M. Cruz). DELTA, 38(3), Article 202258942, 1-27. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678460X202258942.

Campbell, N. & Mokhtari, P. (2003). Voice quality: The 4th prosodic dimension. In M. J. Solé, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2417–2420). https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/ icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2003/papers/p15_2417.pdf.

Castro, B. S. A., Martins-Reis, V. O., Baptista, A. C., & Celeste, L. C. (2014). Fluency profile: Comparison between Brazilian and European Portuguese speakers. CoDAS, 26(6), 444-446. https://doi. org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184.

Cavalcante, M. C. B. & Barros, A. T. M. C. (2012). Manhês: Qualidade vocal e deslocamentos na dialogia mãe-bebê. Veredas – Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 16(spe), 25-39. https://periodicos.ufjf.br/index. php/veredas/article/view/25054.

Chiang, C., Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. H. (2018). Distinguishing questions and statements using sentence-initial prosodic cues. In A. B. Bertolini & M. J. Kaplan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 153-158). http://www. lingref.com/bucld/42/BUCLD42-12.pdf.

Cinelli, E. (2021, September 17). How prosody plays a role in language development. Verywell Family. https://www.verywellfamily.com/ prosody-and-language-acquisition-5191264.

Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584-1595. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130766.

Cristia, A. (2013). Input to language: The phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(3), 157-170. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12015.

Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2019). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development, 90(3), 759-773. https://doi.org/10.1111/ cdev.12974.

Dadalto, E. V., & Goldfeld, M. (2006). Características do maternalês em duas crianças de idades distintas. Distúrbios da Comunicação, 18(2), 201-208. https://revistas.pucsp.br/index.php/dic/article/ view/11786/8520.

De Looze, C., & Hirst, D. (2014). The OMe (octave median) scale: A natural scale for speech prosody. In N. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 910-914). https://www.isca-speech.org/ archive_v0/SpeechProsody_2014/pdfs/175.pdf.

De Pablo, I, Murillo, E., & Romero, A. (2020). The effect of infant-directed speech on early multimodal communicative production in Spanish and Basque. Journal of Child Language, 47(2), 457-471. https://doi. org/10.1017/S0305000919000412.

Faria, B. S., Oliveira, K. V., Godoy e Silva, J. P., Reis, C., Ghio, A., & Gama, A. C. C. (2012). Electroglottography of speakers of Brazilian Portuguese through objective multiparameter vocal assessment (EVA). Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology, 78(4), 29-34. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1808-86942012000400007.

Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press.

Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(3), 279- 293. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8.

Fernald, A., & O’Neill, D. K. (1993). Peekaboo across cultures: How mothers and infants play with voices, faces, and expectations. In K. MacDonald (Ed.), Parent–child play: Descriptions and implications (pp. 259–285). State University of New York Press.

Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 104–113. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104.

Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0305000900010679.

Floccia, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., DePaolis, R., Duffy, H., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., White, L., Goslin, J., & Vihman, M. (2016). British English infants segment words only with exaggerated infant-directed speech stimuli. Cognition, 148, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cognition.2015.12.004.

Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M. (2014). Infant’s perception of intonation: Is it a statement or a question? Infancy, 19(2), 194-213. https://doi. org/10.1111/infa.12037.

Garnica, O. K. (1977) Some prosodic and paralinguistics features of speech to young children. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 63-88). Cambridge University Press.

Garnica, O. K. (1974). Some prosodic characteristics of speech to young children. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Stanford University.

Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. (2017). Prosodic differences between declaratives and interrogatives in infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language, 44(4), 968-994. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000349.

Gobl, C., & Chasaide, A. N. (2003). The role of voice quality in communicating emotion, mood and attitude. Speech Communication, 40(1-2), 189–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00082-1.

Gout, A., Christophe, A., & Morgan, J. L. (2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access: II Infant data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 548-567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jml.2004.07.002.

Graf Estes, K., & Hurley, K. (2013). Infant-directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy, 18(5), 797-824. https:// doi.org/10.1111/infa.12006.

Grichkovtsova, I., Morel, M., & Lacheret, A. (2012). The role of voice quality and prosodic contour in affective speech perception. Speech Communication, 54(3), 414-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.10.005.

Grieser, D. L., & Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 14-20. https://doi. org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.

Guimarães, I., & Abberton E. (2005). Fundamental frequency in speakers of Portuguese for different voice samples. Journal of Voice, 19(4), 592-606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.11.004.

Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616983. Guzman, M., Correa, S., Muñoz, D., & Mayerhoff, R. (2013). Influence

on spectral energy distribution of emotional expression. Journal of Voice, 27(1), Article 129.e1-129.e10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jvoice.2012.08.008.

Hart, J., Collier, R., & Cohen, A. (1990). A perceptual study of intonation: An experimental-phonetic approach to speech melody. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627743.

Hedberg,N.,Sosa,J.M.,Görgülu,̈ E.,&Mameni,M.(2010).Prosodyand pragmatics of wh-interrogatives. In M. Heijl (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2010 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2010/CLA2010_ Hedberg_et_al.pdf.

Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2001). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non- tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development, 24(4), 372-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3.

Klatt, D. H., & Klatt, L.C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87(2), 820-857. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.398894.

Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808814. Laver, J. (1980). The phonetic description of voice quality. Cambridge

University Press.

Liberman, M. (2018, November 26). A better way to calculate pitch range.

Language Log. https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=40788. Martins, V. O., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2008). Perfil evolutivo da fluência da fala de falantes do português brasileiro. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 20(1), 7-12. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002.

Matsuoka, A., & Name, C. (2011). O uso de pistas prosódicas na identificação do adjetivo por crianças e adultos falantes do PB. In Anais do VII Congresso Internacional da ABRALIN (pp. 577-587).

Meireles, A. R., & Barbosa, P. A. (2008). Speech rate effects on speech rhythm. In Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 327–330). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/ meireles08_speechprosody.pdf.

Meireles, A. R., Tozetti, J. P., & Borges, R. R. (2010). Speech rate and rhythmic variation in Brazilian Portuguese. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2010. Paper 875. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/ sp2010/papers/sp10_875.pdf.

Miranda, L., Moraes, J., & Rilliard, A. (2022). Effects of F0 movements, intensity, and duration in the perceptual identification of Brazilian Portuguese wh-questions and wh-exclamations. Special issue Prosody and interfaces (Ed. by C. Serra, F. Fernandes-Svartman, and M. Cruz). DELTA, 38(3), Article 202258882, 1-29. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258882.

Moraes, J. A. (2008). The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: Analysis by synthesis. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 389-397). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/ moraes08_speechprosody.pdf.

Name, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020a). The prosody of questions in Brazilian Portuguese infant-directed speech. In N. Minematsu, M. Kondo, T. Arai & R. Hayashi (Eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2020 (pp. 334-337). https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-68.

Name, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.

Narayan, C. R., & McDermott, L. C. (2016). Speech rate and pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech: Longitudinal and cross- linguistic observations. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139, 1272-1281. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4944634.

Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp.109-149). Cambridge University Press.

Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2003). Pitch characteristics of speech during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions. The Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40(6), 663-674. https://doi.org/10.6033/ tokkyou.40.663.

Papoušek, M., Papoušek, H., & Haekel, M. (1987). Didactic adjustments in fathers’ and mothers’ speech to their 3-month-old infants. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 491-516. https://doi.org/10.1007/ BF01073274.

Peter, V., Kalashnikova, M., Santos, A., & Burnham, D. (2016). Mature neural responses to infant-directed speech but not adult-directed speech in pre-verbal infants. Nature Scientfic Reports, 6, Article 34273. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34273.

Räsänen, O., Kakouros, S., & Soderstrom, M. (2018). Is infant-directed speech interesting because it is surprising? Linking properties of IDS to statistical learning and attention at the prosodic level. Cognition, 178, 193-206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.015.

Rietveld, T., & Vermillion, P. (2003). Cues for perceived pitch register. Phonetica, 60(4), 261–272. https://doi.org/10.1159/000076376. Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui,

A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pone.0078103.

Silva, V. (1999). Análise eletroglotográfica de diferentes tipos de vozes [Trabalho de conclusão de curso]. Centro de Estudos da Voz.

Silva, I. O., & Name, C. (2014). A sensibilidade de bebês brasileiros a pistas prosódicas de fronteiras de sintagma entoacional na fala dirigida à criança. Letrônica, 7(1), 4-25. https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-4301.2014.1.16855.

Snow, E. C. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31-49). Cambridge University Press.

Snow, E. C. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Finetuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. Fletcher, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180-193). Blackwell.

Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Development Review, 27(4), 501-532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002.

Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. (2008).Acoustic cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 35(4), 869-902. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763.

Soley, G., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2020). Infants’ expectations about the recipients of infant-directed and adult-directed speech. Cognition, 198, 104214, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104214.

Sosa, J. M. (2003). Wh-questions in Spanish: Meanings and configuration variability. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 2, 229-247. https://raco.cat/index.php/CatalanJournal/article/view/308981.

Splendore, K. M., Constantini, A. C., & Da Silva, K. C. (2019). Investigação da prosódia e da linguagem na interação mãe-bebê. Working Papers em Linguística, 20(1), 172-188. https://doi.org/10.5007/19848420.2019v20n1p172.

Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 53-71. https:// doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.

Thorson, J., Borras-Comes, J., Crespo-Sendra, V., Vanrell, M., & Prieto, P. (2014). The acquisition of melodic form and meaning in yes-no interrogatives by Catalan and Spanish children. Probus, 27(1), 73-99. https://doi.org/10.1515/probus-2013-0019.

Wang, Y., Seidl, A., & Cristia, A. (2016). Acoustic characteristics of infant- directed speech as a function of prosodic typology. In J. Heinz, R. Goedemans, & H. Van der Hulst (Eds.), Dimensions of phonological stress (pp. 311-326). Cambridge University Press. https://doi. org/10.1017/9781316212745.012.

Weber, A., Fernald, A., & Diop, Y. (2017). When cultural norms discourage talking to babies: Effectiveness of a parenting program in Rural Senegal. Child Development, 88(5), 1513-1526. https://doi. org/10.1111/cdev.12882.

Weirich, M., & Simpson, A. (2019). Effects of gender, parental role, and time on infant- and adult-directed read and spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(11), 4001-4014. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S190047.

Publicado

2023-09-09

Como Citar

Name, M. C. L., & Sosa, J. M. (2023). A Fala dirigida à criança em português brasileiro:: A prosódia de perguntas . DELTA: Documentação E Estudos Em Linguística Teórica E Aplicada, 38(3). https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-460x202258879